Although the International Dyslexia Association has yet to adopt a new definition for dyslexia, some are considering other examples such as this BDA definition to help think through what may need to be changed in the IDA definition. It is sensible here to point out that the BDA and IDA definitions differ in a few areas. One difference is that the BDA definition provides the more complete description of the characteristic features of dyslexia. Describing these features as “difficulties in phonological awareness, verbal memory, and verbal processing speed,” the definition recognizes a broader picture of varying cognitive profiles that students with dyslexia may exhibit.
The BDA definition also states that dyslexia is “best thought of as a continuum.” This also is an important feature of dyslexia that helps those using this definition to continue to research how interventions and supports can be crafted differently for those with mild, moderate, or severe cases of dyslexia.
The next sentence in the BDA definition addresses an important fact: Difficulties in language, difficulties with attention and motor coordination, and math disability co-occur, often with the most severe cases of dyslexia. Snowling, Hulme, and Nation (2020) state, “None of these comorbidities should be viewed as ‘core’ features of dyslexia, but they can complicate both its presentation and response to intervention” (p. 506).
The last sentence of the BDA definition recognizes that the way to differentiate dyslexia from more “garden-variety” reading difficulties is to see how children respond to “well-founded” intervention. “Well-founded” interventions are those that are aligned to the evidence-base. If a child responds positively to intervention and the reading difficulties have been remediated, then it is not correct to diagnose the child with dyslexia. However, if the child does not respond to the intervention or responds less robustly, this then speaks to the persistence of the problem, which is a clear marker for dyslexia. Intervention approaches should address co-occurring difficulties that may require additional instructional intervention but also mental health interventions to address co-occurring depression or Attention Deficit Disorder that is commonly present among students with dyslexia.
Reading scientists continue to research dyslexia but there is still much to be learned. We know that a phonological deficit is quite often present in students with dyslexia and that interventions that address this deficit have proved successful for the vast majority of students. However, researchers have been questioning whether a phonological deficit is a causal factor for dyslexia or a symptom of it. We also know that different orthographies influence decoding accuracy and fluency in different ways. This knowledge is important to understand and continue to build upon. More research is needed to uncover this question. A new and updated definition can help move the needle toward more accurate ways to diagnose and intervene for dyslexia.
References
Brady, S. (2019). The 2003 IDA Definition of dyslexia: A call for changes. Perspectives on Language and Literacy, 45(1). International Dyslexia Association.
Fletcher, J. M., Lyon, G. R., Fuchs, L. S., & Barnes, M. A. (2019). Learning disabilities: From identification to intervention (2nd ed.). New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Hulme, C., Nash, H. M., Gooch, D., Lervåg, A., & Snowling, M. J. (2015). The foundations of literacy development in children at familial risk of dyslexia. Psychological Science, 26(12), 1877–1886. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797615603702
Lyon, G. R., Shaywitz, S. E., & Shaywitz, B. A. (2003). A definition of dyslexia. Annals of Dyslexia, 53, 1–14.
Nation, K., & Snowling, M. J. (1998). Semantic processing and the development of word-recognition skills: Evidence from children with reading comprehension difficulties. Journal of Memory and Language, 39(1), 85–101. https://doi.org/10.1006/jmla.1998.2564